Примечания

1

Способность ребенка рассматривать окружающую среду с позиции, отличной от той, в которой находится он сам (например, с позиции куклы, сидящей где-то в стороне). – Прим, перее.

Комментарии

1

McGurk, Н. & Macdonald, J. Heraring lips and seeing voices. Nature 264, 691–811 (1976).

2

Chartrand, T.L. & Bargh, J.A. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 16. 893–910 (1999).

3

Gallup, A.C. et al. Visual attention and the acquisition of information in human crowds. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 109, 7245–7250 (2012).

Farkas, I., Helbing, D. & Vicsek, T. Human waves in stadiums. In Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications 330, 18–24 (2003).

4

Chartrand, T.L. & Bargh, J.A. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 16. 893–910 (1999).

5

Carp, J., Halenar, M.J., Quandt, L.C., Slclar, A. & Compton, R.J. Perceived similarity and neural mirroring: Evidence from vicarious error processing. Soc. Neurosci. 4, 85–96 (2009).

6

Wang, Y., Ramsey, R. & Hamilton, A.F. de C. The control of mimicry by eye contact is mediated by medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 12001-10 (2011).

7

Chartrand, T.L. & Bargh, J.A. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 76. 893–910 (1999).

8

Harrison, N.A., Singer, T., Rotshtein, P., Dolan, R.J. & Critchley, H.D. Pupillary contagion: central mechanisms engaged in sadness processing. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 1, 5-17 (2006).

9

Codrons, E., Bernardi, N.F., Vandoni, M. & Bernardi, L. Spontaneous group synchronization of movements and respiratory rhythms. PLoS One 9, (2014).

10

Meltzoff, A.N. & Moore, M.K. Imitation of Facial and Manual Gestures by Human Neonates. Science (80-.). 198, 75–78 (1977).

11

Gazzola, V. et al. Aplasics Born without Hands Mirror the Goal of Hand Actions with Their Feet. Curr. Biol 17, 1235–1240 (2007).

12

Cross, E.S., Hamilton, A.F. de C. & Grafton, S.T. Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage 31, 1257–1267 (2006).

13

Lahav, A., Saltzman, E. & Schlaug, G. Action Representation of Sound: Audiomotor Recognition Network While Listening to Newly Acquired Actions. J. Neurosci. 27, 308–314 (2007).

14

Buccino, G. et al. The neural basis for understanding non-intended actions. Neuroimage 36, (2007).

15

Gallese, V. Keysers, C. & Rizzolatti, G. A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 396–403 (2004).

16

Pelphrey, K.A., Morris, J. R & McCarthy, G. Grasping the intentions of others: the perceived intentionality of an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1706–1716. (2004).

17

Catmur, C., Walsh, V. & Heyes, C. Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System. Curr. Biol 17, 1527–1531 (2007).

18

Kohler, E. et al. Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science 297, 846-8 (2002).

19

Bailey, RE. & Henry, J.D. Subconscious facial expression mimicry is preserved in older adulthood. Psychol Aging 24, 995-1000 (2009).

20

Oberman, L.M., Winkielman, P. & Ramachandran, V.S. Face to face: blocking facial mimicry can selectively impair recognition of emotional expressions. Soc. Neurosci 2, 167–178 (2007).

21

Davis, J.I., Senghas, A. Brandt, F. & Ochsner, K.N. The effects of BOTOX injections on emotional experience. Emotion 10, 433–440 (2010).

22

Lakin, J.L. & Chartrand, T.L. Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol Sci. 14, 334–339 (2003).

23

Zajonc, R.B., Adelmann, P.K., Murphy, S.T. & Niedenthal, P.M. Convergence in the physical appearance of spouses. Motiv. Emot. 11, 335–346 (1987).

24

Chartrand, T.L. & Bargh, J.A. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 16. 893–910 (1999).

25

Lakin, J.L., Chartrand, T.L. & Arkin, R.M. I am too just like you. Nonconscious mimicry as an automatic beavioral response to social exclusion. Psychol Sci. 19, 816–822 (2008).

26

van Baaren, R.B., Maddux, W.W., Chartrand, T.L. de Bouter, C. & van Knippenberg, A. It takes two to mimic: Behavioral consequences of self construals. J. Pers. Soc, Psychol. 84, 1093–1102 (2003).

27

Neal, D.T. & Chartrand, T.L. Embodied Emotion Perception: Amplifying and Dampening Facial Feedback Modulates Emotion Perception Accuracy. Soc. Psychol Personal Sci. (2011). doi: 10.1177/ 1948550611406138

28

Schachter, S. & Singer, J. Cognitive, social and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychol Rev. 69(5). 379–399 (1962).

29

Chartrand, T.L. & Bargh, J.A. The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 76. 893–910 (1999).

30

Jabbi, M., Swart, M. & Keysers, C. Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. Neuroimage 34, 1744–1753 (2007).

31

Simner, M.L. Newborn’s response to the cry of another infant. Dev. Psychol. 5, 136–150 (1971).

32

Dondi, M., Simion, F. & Caltran, G. Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant? Dev. Psychol. 35, 418 (1999).

33

Preston, S.D. & De Waal, F. Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav. Brain Sci. 25, 1-20 (2002).

34

Strack, F., Martin, F.F. & Stepper, S. Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 768–777 (1988).

35

Hopp, H., Rohrmann, S., Zapf, D. & Hodapp, V. Psychophysiologi-cal effects of emotional dissonance in a face-to-face service inter-acion. Anxiety. Stress. Coping 23, 399–414 (2010).

36

Zapf, D., Seifert, C., Schmutte, B., Mertini, H. & Holz, M. Emotion work and job stressors and their effects on burnout. Psychol. Health 16, 527–545 (2001).

37

Masserman, J.H., Wechkin, S. & Terris, W. «Altruistic» behavior in rhesus monkeys. Am. J. Psychiatry 121, 584–585 (1964).

38

Jackson, P.E., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A.N. & Decety, J. Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain. Neuropsychologia 44, 752–761 (2006).

39

Zillmann, D., Weaver, J.B., Mundorf, N. & Aust, C.F. Effects of an opposite-gender companion’s affect to horrof on distress, delight, and attraction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 586–594 (1986).

40

Singer, T. & Klimecki, O.M. Empathy and compassion. Curr. Biol. 24, R875-R878 (2014).

41

Silani, G., Lamm, C., Ruff, C.C. & Singer, T. Right supramarginal gyrus is crucial to overcome emotional egcentricity bias in social judgments. J. Neurosci. 33, 15466-15476 (2013).

42

Ruby, R & Decety, J. How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with social emotions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 988–999 (2004).

43

Ruby, R & Decety, J. How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with social emotions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 988–999 (2004).

44

Hoffmann, E, Singer, T. & Steinbeis, N. Children’s Increased Emotional Egocentricity Compared to Adults is Mediated by Age-Related Differences in Conflict Processing. Child Dev. 86, 765–780 (2015).

45

Riva, F., Triscoli, C., Lamm, C., Camaghi, A. & Silani, G. Emotional Egocentricity Bias Across the Life-Span. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8, (2016).

46

Savitsky, K., Keysar, B., Epley, N., Carter, T. & Swanson, A. The closeness-communication bias: Increased egocentrism among friends versus strangers. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 47, 129–273 (2011).

47

Ruby, R & Decety, J. How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with social emotions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 988–999 (2004).

48

Lamm, C., Batson, C. D. & Decety, J. The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J. Cogn. Nerosci. 19, 42–58 (2007).

49

Gu, X. & Han, S. Attention and reality constraints on the neural processes of empathy for pain. Neuroimage 36, 256–267 (2007).

50

Bavel, J.J. Van Packer, D.J. & Cunningham, W.A. The Neural Substrates of In-Group Bias. Psychol. Sci. (2008).

51

Kim, M.J. et al. The structural and functional connectivity of the amygdala: From normal emotion tho pathological anxiety. Behav. Brain Res. 223, 403–410 (2011).

52

Gross, J.J. Emotion Regulation: Conceptual and Empirical Foundations. Handbook of Emotion Regulation 3-20 (2014). doi:10.1080/ 00140130600971135

53

Koenigsberg, H.W. et al. Neural correlates of using distancing to regulate emotional responses to social situations. Neuropsychologia 48, 1813–1822 (2010).

54

Goldin, P.R., McRae, K., Ramel W. & Gross, J.J. The Neural Bases of Emotion Regulation: Reappraisal and Suppression of Negative Emotion. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 577–586 (2008).

55

Richards, J.M. & Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation and memory: the cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 410–424 (2000).

56

Banks, S.J., Eddy, K.T., Angstadt, M., Nathan, P.J. & Luan Phan, K. Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 303–312 (2007).

57

Kim, M.J., Gee, D.G., Loucks, R.A., Davis, F.C. & Whalen, P.J. Anxiety Dissociates dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex functional connectivity with the amygdala at rest, Cereb. Cortex 21, 1667–1673 (2011).

58

Giuliani, N.R., Drabant, E.M., Bhatnagar, R. & Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation and brain plasticity: Expressive suppression use predicts anterior insula volume. Neuroimage 58, 10–15 (2011).

59

Hayes, J.R et al. Staying cool when things get hot: emotion regulation modulates meural mechanisms of memory encoding. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4, 230 (2010).

60

Richards, J.M., Butler, E.A. & Gross, J.J. Emotion Regulation in Romantic Relationships: The Cognitive Consequences of Concealing Feelings. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 20, 599–620 (2003).

61

Eippert, F. et al. Regulation of emotional repsonses elicited by threat-related stimuli. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 409–423 (2007).

62

Tangney, J.R, Baumeister, R.F. & Boone, A.L. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J. Pers. 72, 271–324 (2004).

63

Batson, C.D., Duncan, B.D., Ackerman, R, Buckley, T. & Birch, K. Is empathic emotion a source of altruistic motivation? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 40, 290 (1981).

64

Adams, Richard E., Boscarino, Joseph A., Figley, C.R. Compassion Fatigue and Psychological Distress Among Social Workers: A Validation Study Access. NIHPublic Access 16, 103–108 (2006).

Rossi, A. et al. Burnout, compassion fatigue, and compassion satisfaction among staff in community-based mental health services. Psychiatry Res. 200, 933–938 (2012).

Yoder, E.A. Compassion fatigue in nurses. Appl. Nurs. Res. 23, 191–197 (2010).

El-Bar, N., Levy, A., Wald, H.S. & Biderman, A. Compassion fatigue, burnout and compassion satisfaction among family physicians in the Negev area – a cross-sectional study. Isr. J. Health Policy Res. 2, 31 (2013).

65

Vachon, D.D., Lynam, D.R. & Johnson, J.A. The (non)relation between empathy and aggression: surprising results from a metaanalysis. Psychol. Bull. 140, 751-73 (2014).

66

Roseman, I.J., Spindel, M.S. & Jose, RE. Appraisals of emotion-eliciting events: Testing a theory of discrete emotions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 59, 899–915 (1990).

67

Bandura, A., Caprara, G.V., Barbaranelli, C., Gerbino M. & Pas-torelli, C. Role of affective self-regulatory efficacy in diverse sprhe-res of psychosocial functioning. Child Dev. 74, 769–782 (2015).

Rossetto, K.R., Lannutti, P.J. & Smith, R.A. Investigating self-effi-cacy and emotional challenge as contributors to willingness to provide emotional support. South. Commun. J. 79, 41–58 (2014).

68

Singer, T. & Klimecki, O.M. Empathy and compassion. Curr. Biol. 24, R875-R878 (2014).

69

Batson, C.D., O’Quin, K., Fultz, J., Vanderplas, M & Isen, A.M. Influence of self-reported dstress and empathy on egoistic versus altruistic motivation to help. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 45, 706 (1983).

70

Tarr, B., Launay, L., Cohen, E. & Dunbar, R. Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biol. Lett. 11, 0–3 (2015).

71

Dunbar, R.I.M. et al. Social laughter is correlated with an elevated pain threshold. Proc. R. Soc. Biol. Sci. 279, 1161–1167 (2012).

72

Singer, T. & Bolz, M. Compassion: bridging practice and science. (Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 2013).

73

Klimecki, O.M., Leiberg, S., Ricard, M. & Singer, T. Differential pattern of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Nerosci. 9, 873–879 (2014).

Klimecki, O.M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C. & Singer, T. Functional neural plasticity and associated changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cereb. Cortex (New York, N.Y. 1991) 23, 1552–1561 (2013).

74

Bogerts, B., Hzjntsch, J. T Herzer, M. A morphometric sutdy of the dopamine-containing cell groups in the mesencephalon of normals, Parkinson patients, and schizophrenics. Biol. Psychiatry (1983).

75

Kirley, A. et al. Dopaminergic system genes in ADHD: Toward a biological hypothesis. Neuropsychopharmacology 27, 607–619 (2002).

Abi-Dargham, A. & Morre, H. Prefrontal DA Transmission at D1 Receptors and the Pathology of Schizophrenia. Neurosci. 9, 404–416 (2003).

Dauer, W. & Przedborski, S. Parkinson’s disease: Mechanisms and models. Neuron 39, 889–909 (2003).

76

Roesch, M.R., Calu, D.J. & Schoenbaum, G. Dopamine neurons encode the better option in rats deciding between delayed or sized rewards. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1615–1624 (2007).

77

Roesch, M.R., Calu, D.J. & Schoenbaum, G. Dopamine neurons encode the better option in rats deciding between delayed or sized rewards. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1615–1624 (2007).

78

Kringelbach, M.L., O’Doherty, J., Rolls, E.T. & Andrews, C. Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1064–1071 (2003).

79

O’Doherty, J. et al. Sensory-specific satiety-related olfactory activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroreport 11, 893–897 (2000).

80

Kim, J.-W. et al. Compassionate attitude towards others’ suffering activates the mesolimbic neural system. Neuropsychologia 47, 2073–2081 (2009).

81

Salimpoor, V.N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A. & Zatorre, R.J. Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 257–262 (2011).

82

Bems, G.S. & Moore, S.E. A neural predictor of cultural popularity. J. Consum. Psychol. 22, 154–160 (2012).

83

Moll, J. et al. Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 15623-8 (2006).

84

Tusche, A. & Вцск1ег, A. Decoding the Charitable Brain: Empathy, Perspective Taking, and Attention Shifts Diffferentially Predict Altruistic Giving. J. Neurosci. (2016).

85

Harbaugh, W.T., Mayr, U. & Burghart, D.R. Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science 316, 1622–1625 (2007).

86

McCall, C. & Singer, T. The animal and human neuroendocrinology of social cognition, motivation and behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 681–688 (2012).

Nelson, E.E. & Panksepp, J. Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 22, 437–452 (1998).

Numan, M. Motivational systems and the neural circuitry of maternal behavior in the rat. Developmental Psychobiology 49, 12–21 (2007).

87

von Hofsten, О. et al. Simulating newborn face perception. J. Vis. 14, 1–9 (2014).

88

Pileggi, L.-A., Malcolm-Smith, S. & Solms, M. Investigating the role of social-affective attachment processes in cradling bias: The absence of cradling bias in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Laterality 1-17 (2014). doi: 10.1080/1357650X2014948449

89

Sieratzki, J.S. & Woll, B. Neuropsychological and neuropsychiatric perspectives on maternal cradling preferences. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Soc. 11, 170–176 (2002).

90

Koda, H. & Masataka, N. A pattern of common acoustic modification by human mothers to gain attention of a child and by macaques of others in their group. Psychol. Rep. 91, 421–422 (2002).

91

Brosnan, S.F. & De Waal, F.B.M. A Proximate Perspective on Reciprocal Altruism. Hum. Nat. 13, 129–152 (2002).

92

Romero, T. Castellanos, M. & de Waal, F.B.M. Consolation as possible expression of sympathetic concern among chimpanzees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 12110-5 (2010).

93

Murdock, G.K., Stine, W.W. & Maple, T.L. Observations of maternal-infant interactions in a captive herd of sable antelope (Hippo-tragus niger). Zoo Biol. 2, 215–224 (1983).

94

Mikulincer, M., Shaver, PR., Gillath, O. & Nitzberg, R.A. Attachment, caregiving, and altruism: boosting attachment security incrtea-ses compassion and helping. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 817 (2005).

95

Kurzban, R. The social psychophysics of cooperation: Nonverbal communication in a public goods game. J. Nonverbal Behav. 25, 241–259 (2001).

96

Nelson, E.E. & Panksepp, J. Brain substrates of infant-mother attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 22, 437–452 (1998).

Bornemann, В., Кок, B.E., Вцск1ег, A. & Singer T. Helping from the heart: Voluntary upregulation of heart rate variability predicts altruistic behavior. Biol. Psychol. 119, 54–63 (2016).

97

Williams, L.E. & Bargh, J.A. Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science 322, 606–607 (2008).

Kang, Y., Williams, L.E., Clark, M.S., Gray, J.R. & Bargh J.A. Physical temperature effects on trust behavior: the role of insula. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 507–515 (2011).

98

Lamm, C., Meltzoff, A.N. & Decety, J. How do we empathize with someone who is not like us? A functional magnetic resonance imaging sutdy. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 362–376 (2010).

99

Ruffman, T., Perner, J., Natto, M., Parkin, L. & Clements, W. A. Older (but not younger) siblings facilitate false belief understanding. Dev. Psychol 34, 161–174 (1998).

100

Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhom, M., Richlan, F. & Perner, J. Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 9-34 (2014).

101

Saxe, R. & Powell, L.J. It’s the thought that counts: specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychol. Sci. 17, 692–699 (2006).

102

Van Overwalle, F. Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 829–858 (2009).

103

Van Overwalle, F. Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 829–858 (2009).

104

Liu, D., Gelman, S.A. & Wellman, H.M. Components of young children’s trait understanding: Behavior-to-trait inferences and trait-to-behavior predictions. Child. Dev. 78, 1543–1558 (2007).

105

Behrens, T.E.J.T. Hunt, L.T.L., Woolrich, M.M.W. & Rushworth, M.F.S. Associative learning of social value. Nature 456, 245–249 (2008).

106

Garcia-Marques, T. & Mackie, D.M. Familiarity impacts person perception. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 37, 839–855 (2007).

107

Alvarez, J.A. & Emory, E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review 16, 17–42 (2006),

108

Wang. G.J. et al. Exposure to appetitive food stimuli markedly activates the human brain. Neuroimage 21, 1790–1797 (2004).

109

Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G.L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215 (2002).

110

Overwalle, F. Van & Baetens, K. Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage 48, 564–584 (2009).

111

Woodward, A. L. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition 69, 1-34 (1998).

112

Johnson, S.C., Slaughter, V. & Carey, S. Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds. Dev. Sci. 1, 233–238 (1998).

113

Gergely, G.N., N6dasdy Z., Csibra G. & Biry S.. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56, 165–193 (1995).

114

Saxe, R., Carey, S. & Kanwisher, N. Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annu. Rev. Psychol 55, 87-124 (2004).

115

Buccino, G. et al. The neural basis for understanding non-intended actions. Neuroimage 36, (2007).

116

Flombaum, J.I. & Santos, L.R. Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others. Curr. Biol 15, 447–452 (2005).

117

Hegel, E, Krach, S. & Kircher, T. Theory of Mind (ToM) on robots: a functional neuroimaging study. Human-Robot Interact. 335–342 (2008). doi: 10.1145/1349822.1349866

118

Grossman, E.D. & Blake, R. Brain areas active during visual perception of biological motion. Neuron 35, 1167–1175 (2002).

119

Riekki, T., Lindeman, M. & Raij, T.T. Supernatural believers attribute more intentions to random movement than sceptics: An fMRI study. Soc. Neurosci. 9, 400–411 (2014).

120

Miesler, L., Leder, H. & Herrmann, A. Isn’t it cute: An evolutionary perspective of baby-schema effects in visual product designs. Int. J. Des. 5, 17–30 (2011).

121

Van Overwalle, F. Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 829–858 (2009).